Why do you think that understanding cognitive development during middle childhood would be useful for your work and practice? What would you like to discuss in greater detail during class? Explain why you chose this topic.

Applied Life Span Developmental Psychology

Childhood
In the preceding chapters, we have described some of the major developmental tasks
and achievements of infancy and the preschool years. The pace of developmental
progress is quite remarkable during those periods, and much of the foundation for
later achievements is laid down. Consider, for example, the qualitative difference between the skills of a newborn and those of his 6-year-old sibling. Development has
been so rapid during this first 6 years that it is not uncommon for parents with a new
born second child to view their older child as having grown up, almost overnight,
because of physical size, motor skills, and verbal ability. However, in middle child
hood, the years spanning the elementary school period, children are still far from
grown up. Their growth takes on new forms in this stage, metamorphosing into ever
more highly differentiated patterns of cognitive, emotional, and social functioning.
For children at the start of the middle years, school and its peer group structure
represent a new frontier. The movement into the school years ushers in a whole
new set of developmental challenges for children. Many youngsters begin to spend
longer periods away from home, and all children must adjust to more rigorous
schedules. They must learn to control their behavior, monitor their attention, and
acquire more formal and more complicated academic competencies than have been
formerly attained. They must make friends and learn to navigate the schoolyard,
with its greater demands for athletic prowess, social skill, and cooperative negotia
tion of conflicts. They must also learn the rules of the group and when to abide by
them. They must learn what it means to be male or female, not to mention what it
means to be themselves. So many challenges await them.
However, children at elementary school age are also more adept at almost
every task when compared to their preschool-aged siblings. Observing the eager
ness and energy young children exhibit in the early school years makes it easy to
understand the capacity for industry that Erikson described (see Chapter 1). For
most children, the challenges of school and peer group will be mastered gradually,
202
c h a p t e r

BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR 203

# 150618 Cust: Pearson Au: Broderick Blewitt Pg. No. 203
Title: The Life Span: Human Development for Helping Professionals, 4e Server:
C / M / Y / K/
Short / Normal / Long S4carliSlePublishing Services
and in many different ways. Armed with foundational skills in language, mobility,
understanding of self and others, and self-control, the youngster is now poised to
assume membership in a larger social network.
Clearly, the years of elementary school, as the years to follow, are marked by
ups and downs. These normal fluctuations create opportunities for helpers to pro
vide support or guidance for children, their families, and their teachers. What are the
cognitive, emotional, and social needs of children at this stage? What approaches
are most helpful given children’s developmental level? In the next two chapters, we
will attempt to provide you with information that will be useful when working with
children at this point in their development. Your understanding of cognitive devel
opment, the focus of this chapter, will enable you to understand children’s ways of
construing the world, helping you appreciate their academic needs as well as the in
tellectual bases for their friendships, gender roles, moral understanding, and conflicts.
BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR
Let’s begin by briefly considering the child’s changing brain. Changes in brain size
and organization accompany the accomplishments of middle childhood. It is tempt
ing to assume that these brain changes are “maturational,” that is, triggered by pre-
programmed genetic activity. But remember the epigenetic process when you think
about brain development: “it is the ongoing interaction of the organism and the environment that guides biological . . . development. Brains do not develop normally
in the absence of genetic signaling, and they do not develop normally in the absence
of essential and contingent environmental input” (emphasis added; Stiles, 2009,
pp. 196–197). Put more positively, genes and experience dynamically interact to influ
ence emerging brain organization. There is no simple answer to the question, “Is this
brain/behavioral change genetic or determined by experience?” It is both.
Even though the brain is at 95% of its peak size by age 6, it grows measur
ably in middle childhood (Lenroot et al., 2007; Sowell, Thompson, & Toga, 2004;
see Blakemore, 2012). One of the important contributors to this growth is that white
matter increases in volume, a process that continues well into adulthood. As you
learned in Chapter 2, white matter is “white” because of the fatty myelin sheaths
that form around the axons, insulating them so that electrical impulses travel faster
from one neuron to another. Myelination increases the speed of neural signals dra
matically. Also, functional neural networks become more integrated because myelin
changes the timing and synchrony of neuronal firing (Giedd & Rapoport, 2010).
Overall then, increasing white matter seems to reflect increasing neural connectivity
and communication between neurons and between brain areas.
For example, one important area of white matter increase is the corpus cal
losum. This is the system of connecting fibers (bundles of axons) between the two
hemispheres of the brain. As the corpus callosum myelinates, the left and right
sides of the body become more coordinated. The upshot is that children have much
greater motor control, something you can appreciate if you compare the awkward
full frontal running of a 3-year-old to the ducking and weaving of a 9-year-old as he
avoids capture during a game of tag. Changes in the corpus callosum (along with
other brain areas, like the cerebellum) influence and are influenced by the great
strides school age children make in both gross motor (e.g., riding a bicycle, skating,
climbing trees, jumping rope) and fine motor (e.g., cutting, drawing, writing) skills.
Much of this chapter is focused on typical or normative development in
middle childhood, but there are many individual differences among children. Re
searchers are beginning to link some of these to brain development. For example,
children diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) can
show atypical variations in brain development (e.g., Sowell, Thompson, et al., 2003).
Between 5 and 10 percent of school-age children are diagnosed with ADHD based
on one or more of a cluster of symptoms that are especially problematic for school
BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR 203

204 Chapter 6 • realms of Cognition in middle Childhood
# 150618 Cust: Pearson Au: Broderick Blewitt Pg. No. 204
Title: The Life Span: Human Development for Helping Professionals, 4e Server:
C / M / Y / K/
Short / Normal / Long S4carliSlePublishing Services
performance: poor attentional control, restlessness or hyperactivity, and impulsivity
(e.g., Kessler et al., 2005). Studies comparing structural MRIs for children with and
without ADHD have found differences in several brain areas. These include the
frontal lobes, where normative growth is associated with improvements in attention
and other higher order cognitive processes. Other areas include the parietal lobes,
basal ganglia, corpus callosum, and cerebellum (for detailed reviews see Giedd &
Rapoport, 2010; Krain & Castellanos, 2006). Longitudinal research indicates that
for many children the “difference” is really a delay, especially in the growth of the
cerebral cortex. The middle prefrontal cortex shows the greatest delay, with growth
for ADHD children lagging behind typically developing children by as much as
5 years (e.g., Shaw et al., 2007). Fortunately, about half of ADHD cases diagnosed
in childhood remit by late adolescence or early adulthood. For those children, it appears that brain development follows a delayed but typical trajectory. For cases of
ADHD that do not remit, researchers have found unusual, progressive loss of brain
volume in some brain areas, such as the cerebellum (Mackie et al., 2007). Note that
there is some disagreement about whether ADHD actually comprises more than one
disorder, with different frontal brain areas more affected in one type versus another
(see Diamond, 2005).
Many children not diagnosed with ADHD are behaviorally different from average—they have better or worse attentional control or they are more or less impulsive or active than other children their age. Giedd and Rapoport (2010) sug
gest that “ADHD is best considered dimensionally, lying at the extreme of a con
tinuous distribution of symptoms and underlying cognitive impairments” (p. 730).
In line with this argument, they report that for children who are considered typi
cally developing but more active and impulsive than average, typical brain
changes also take place at a slower rate. Thus, researchers are beginning to
identify some neurological differences among children that align with their
behavioral differences. In general, helpers need to remember that there is
a significant amount of unevenness in brain development in middle child
hood, both between and within children (Berninger & Hart, 1992). It is not
unusual for children to show lagging performance in some skills and more
rapid advances in other skills than their age mates.
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
When children leave behind the preschool years, they begin to seem much
more savvy to adults. They can be given fairly complex responsibilities
(“Take out the dog before you go to school, and don’t forget to lock the
door after you leave.”). They can participate in discussions of local or world
events, and they often appreciate humor that would have been lost on them
earlier. The cognitive developments that underlie these new capacities have
been described and studied from several different theoretical traditions.
We will first present Piaget’s characterization of cognitive change in middle
childhood.
Piaget’s Stage of Concrete Operations
Since the days of Christopher Columbus, we have been teaching children
that the Earth is round. Of course, it looks flat, especially if you live some
where in the midwestern United States, such as Minnesota. How do children
reconcile what they are told with what they perceive? When Vosniadou and
Brewer (1992) asked Minnesota school children in grades 1 through 5 about
this apparent contradiction, they got some surprising answers. Figure 6.1 illustrates a few of them. Some children said the Earth was a flat disc—like a
coin. Some thought it was a ball that has a flat surface within it and a domed
sky overhead. Others saw the Earth as spherical but with a flattened side
(a) (b)
Sphere
Flattened Sphere
Hollow Sphere
Dual Earth
Disc Earth
Rectangular Earth
FIGURE 6.1 Children’s images of a
round Earth.
Source: Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, N. F. (1992).
Mental models of the earth: A study of concep
tual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology,
24, 549. used with permission from elsevier.

Answer the question with short answers (when possible) SEE example question number 1

Friendship understanding refers to a child’s evolving understanding of what friendship entails.
Advances in:
Selective attention
Metacognition
Metamemory
Teacher factors influencing educational outcomes:
Mindset: Growth vs. fixed (ability) mindset
Are “learning styles” real?
Friends as protective factors

Reflection Questions:

Why do you think that understanding cognitive development during middle childhood would be useful for your work and practice?

What would you like to discuss in greater detail during class? Explain why you chose this topic.

What concepts were surprising or confusing? Explain.

 

Why do you think that understanding cognitive development during middle childhood would be useful for your work and practice? What would you like to discuss in greater detail during class? Explain why you chose this topic.
Scroll to top